Quantcast

NYC Gazette

Friday, January 10, 2025

Lemur study reveals role of memory scent & social cues finding food

Webp vfattgaezc76f4j9qzyrc2qqxv4i

Nouriel Roubini, Professor of Economics and International Business at New York University's Stern School of Business | New York University's Stern School of Business

Nouriel Roubini, Professor of Economics and International Business at New York University's Stern School of Business | New York University's Stern School of Business

Two mongoose lemurs find cantaloupe hidden in a container at the Lemur Conservation Foundation in Florida. Photo credit: Elena Cunningham

How do foraging animals find their food? A new study by New York University researchers shows that lemurs use smell, social cues, and long-term memory to locate hidden fruit—a combination of factors that may have deep evolutionary roots.

“Our study provides evidence that lemurs can integrate sensory information with ecological and social knowledge, which demonstrates their ability to consider multiple aspects of a problem,” said anthropologist Elena Cunningham, a clinical professor of molecular pathobiology at NYU College of Dentistry and the lead author of the study, published in the International Journal of Primatology.

Animals rely on their senses and environmental and social knowledge to locate food and water. These factors—perhaps in combination—are thought to have played a role in primates evolving to have larger brains and higher cognitive abilities than other animals.

“Historically, there have been two schools of thought on why primates developed bigger brains: ecological pressures, such as needing to find scarce fruit in the forest, and the social pressures of living in a group where everyone is trying to outsmart one another,” said Cunningham. “I have long been interested in the interplay between social and ecological factors when it comes to cognition—it seems like a given that these would have evolved in relation to each other.”

To better understand how primates integrate these factors in order to find food, Cunningham traveled to the Lemur Conservation Foundation in Myakka City, Florida, a reserve dedicated to researching and protecting lemurs outside of their native Madagascar. The Foundation is home to several lemur species, including brown lemurs—social animals who have a keen sense of smell (far better than humans) and whose diet is primarily fruit.

Studying the brown lemurs in pairs and groups of three, the researchers conducted several experiments by hiding pieces of cantaloupe in cardboard takeout containers and placing both fruit-filled and empty containers in the lemurs’ environments. They then observed how the groups of lemurs investigated and opened the containers, noting their interactions with one another.

A mongoose lemur looks for food in a takeout container at the Lemur Conservation Foundation in Florida. Photo credit: Elena Cunningham

Despite the empty takeout containers outnumbering those with cantaloupe, the lemurs had little trouble finding and eating the fruit—and several factors appeared to be working in tandem.

The lemurs quickly learned which containers had food in them and could remember the location of the fruit-filled containers days, weeks, and even months later: the order in which they approached baited containers was about 50 percent better than chance. But, the lemurs almost always (98 percent of the time) opened the fruit-filled containers first, suggesting that they used their sense of smell to detect cantaloupe at close range.

In addition, researchers observed that individual strategies for finding fruit were influenced by social factors. Some groups were egalitarian with information and melon willingly shared; others were more hierarchical with dominant lemurs taking advantage once subordinates discovered cantaloupe. Subordinates were more likely to find fruit initially but some used their “finder’s advantage” to eat more melon.

“What our study shows is that these three factors are all operating at the same time—the lemurs have memories of where food is while considering olfactory information and social factors,” said Cunningham. “We still have much to learn about this interplay and evolution of cognition but it's important not to look at these factors isolation.”

Additional study authors include Malvin Janal, Rachelle Wolk, and Maria Gonzalez-Robles from NYU Dentistry. The research was supported by NYU Research Challenge Fund Program along with NYU College of Dentistry Academy Distinguished Educators Funding Award.

___

!RECEIVE ALERTS

The next time we write about any of these orgs, we’ll email you a link to the story. You may edit your settings or unsubscribe at any time.
Sign-up

DONATE

Help support the Metric Media Foundation's mission to restore community based news.
Donate